|
I. Introduction ...................................................................................................................1A. Statement of the Problem....................................................................................... 1B. Review of Literature............................................................................................... 41. Make it Voluntary............................................................................................. 42. Elements of Reading with Understanding ........................................................43. Direct Instruction............................................................................................. 54. The Importance of Phonics and Explicit Phonics ............................................65. Usefulness of Immediate Feedback on Errors ...............................................116. Teaching Blending, Letter Sounds, and Sounding Out.................................. 117. Direct Instruction in Word Meanings (Vocabulary)...................................... 118. Explicit Instruction in Logic......................................................................... 129. Orton Phonograms ......................................................................................1210. Usefulness of Dictation; the Spalding Spelling Rules................................ 12C. Statement of the Hypothesis ............................................................................13II. The Phonics Institute "Every Teacher a Reading Teacher" Reading and WritingSchool Improvement Program.............................................................................13 A. Description of the Proposal........................................................................... 13B. Comments on the Proposal ..........................................................................15References .............................................................................................................16
"Every Teacher a Reading Teacher" Phonics-based Reading and Writing School Improvement Project I. Introduction Pulitzer Prize-winning journalist Melvin Claxton wrote a series of articles for the Virgin Islands Daily News in December, 1996. The series was a six-part special report entitled "Cheating our Children: How Education Officials Hide Their Failures" (Claxton, 1996). The articles detailed how poorly many of our Virgin Islands public school students are performing in reading, writing, and math, and detailed a host of other problems with our Virgin Islands public education system. Claxton reported on the 1994 Metropolitan Achievement Test, Seventh Edition (MAT7), results for Virgin Islands public school students. The results revealed that the reading ability of 93% of Virgin Islands public school grade 7 students were below grade level; for grade 8, 79% were below grade level; for grade 9, 88% were below grade level; and for grades 10, 11, and 12, 73%, 86%, and 78% of the students were reading below grade level.The author of this school improvement plan is concerned about the poor reading abilities of many of our Virgin Islands public school students. The author decided to conduct an investigation at the John H. Woodson Junior High School in St. Croix, part of the Virgin Islands public school system, in order to devise a school improvement plan that may be implemented at the John H. Woodson Junior High School (and other schools) to help the students become better readers and writers. The proposed school-based on-site investigation was approved in writing by Dr. Denis Griffith, the author’s professor in the graduate class known as Fundamentals of Administration, EDU 547, at the St. Croix Campus of the University of the Virgin Islands; and the on-site investigation was approved in writing by the principal of John H. Woodson Junior High School, Vaughn Hewitt, and by Lorraine Gibbs, the St. Croix District Insular Superintendent. The investigation consisted of telephone calls and in-person meetings with Mr. Hewitt; brief personal meetings with the two assistant principals, Gary Molloy and Doris Brodhurst; a joint meeting with all three school counselors; a meeting with Ms Dominique (a reading teacher) and Ms Saddler, an English teacher; a brief meeting with Charles Fisher, a science teacher; four (some extended) meetings with Ms Charlene Matthew, the chairperson of the Reading Department (one attended by Ms Dominique); observation of a reading class conducted by Ms Matthew; an analysis of 1996 7th-grade student Stanford reading test results; a review of a reading class textbook; and participation in the hardwiring of a portion of a computer network on Net Day, which provided some sense of the John H. Woodson, and Virgin Islands public school professional community. What did the investigation reveal? First of all, from my brief exposure to the teachers and other professional staff at John H. Woodson, in meetings, at Net Day, and while milling about together in the parking lot during a bomb scare, there is no doubt the professional staff is capable of the professional development that will be required of them to implement the proposal. We do not need to worry about that. Second, we have a measurement of the reading ability of students entering Woodson. Some read quite well, but, as would be expected given the over-all Virgin Islands public school performance on the MAT7, many entering John H. Woodson Junior High School students have problems reading well. At the beginning of each year every entering 7th-grade student is administered two of the three portions of the Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test to determine his or her current reading level. The tests are scored, resulting in Grade Equivalency (G.E.) scores for each child. This school year, the Stanford test was administered on September 6, 1996 to 390 entering 7th-grade students. Sixty-four students scored above G.E. 7.0, which is the beginning of 7th grade. That’s a lot of real-live, flesh-and-blood boys and girls doing well in reading. No wonder so many Virgin Islands public school students get college scholarships. Yet, 326 students, 83.6 percent of the total, scored below G.E. 7.0; 286, or 73.3%, scored below G.E. 6.0. The median score for all students taking the test was G.E. 4.4, equivalent to an average student in the 4th month of 4th grade, well below where they should have been. Please see the bar graph displaying the results of this test, attached hereto as an appendix. So, many John H. Woodson Junior High School students read poorly when they enter the school. Interviews with school personnel confirmed widespread problems with reading including difficulty with "word recognition" and problems with writing well including spelling problems. Word recognition problems with John H. Woodson Junior High School students could be confirmed (or disproven) if deemed advisable by administering to a sample of students an informal reading inventory such as the Reading Competency Test published by the National Right to Read Foundation. The MAT7 results for 9th grade students (reported in the first paragraph above) demonstrate that after Virgin Islands public school students complete junior high school (7th and 8th grade), many still have a major problem with reading well. It is assumed that 9th graders who are graduates of John H. Woodson Junior High School score, on average, similarly to other Virgin Islands public school 9th graders. Because upon entering the John H. Woodson Junior High School so many 7th graders are reading (and writing) poorly, the school has a Reading Department, the purpose of which is to conduct "reading classes" designed to help the lower-performing students to improve their reading and writing abilities. The Department is staffed with three teachers. Two of them teach four reading classes each to 7th grade students. The third teacher, Ms Matthew, is the department chair. In addition to fulfilling her duties as department chair, she teaches two 7th grade reading classes and one 8th grade reading class. Thus, there are ten 7th-grade reading classes with an average of about 15 students per class for a total served student population of approximately 150. Students are selected for participation in the reading classes based upon their G.E. scores on the Stanford reading tests given in the beginning of the year, with the lowest-scoring students being placed in the reading classes. Since 326 of the 390 entering students this school year were reading below grade level, and since only about 150 students were placed in reading classes, this means approximately 176 students who were reading below grade level were not initially assigned to reading classes. (Although this last figure should be lower now, since as of April 10, 1997, John H. Woodson Junior High School was down to a 7th grade class of 358 students.) Furthermore, as pointed out above, the MAT7 9th grade reading scores demonstrate that when Virgin Islands public school students are in 9th grade, 88% of them are still reading below grade level. (The author of this report does not have the specific figures for John H. Woodson Junior High School graduates.) It would appear that not many students move from below-their-grade-level reading to grade-level reading or above during the two years they spend in junior high school. Can something more, something different, be done to bring the reading abilities of John H. Woodson Junior High School students up to and beyond grade level? What about the curriculum already in place? The Virgin Islands English/Language Arts Curriculum Guide (1994) sets forth objectives but does not detail the methods to be used to teach students to read and write well. Ms Matthew uses a variety of teaching materials in her reading classes. The principal text is the series Be a Better Reader (Sixth Edition) by Nila Banton Smith, which is a series of "consumable" soft-bound books having various levels. The Smith volumes use a series of reading selections, each of which is followed by a series of workbook-style exercises designed to develop the students’ reading, analyzing, synthesizing, interpreting, and thinking abilities. The teachers at John H. Woodson Junior High School who are not "reading teachers" - that is, teachers who are "content area" teachers - do not have any school-issued materials that are intended to encourage and assist the teachers in teaching their students how to read and write better. Within the last couple of years, Dr. Sarah Mahurt of the University of the Virgin Islands St. Croix Campus conducted a training program for Virgin Islands public school junior high school reading teachers in order to train them so that they could train "content area" (non-reading class) teachers within their schools to teach their students how to become better readers, while the teachers were at the same time teaching their specific subjects. This attempt at getting "content area" teachers to see themselves as "reading teachers" apparently never amounted to much of anything at John H. Woodson Junior High School. If John H. Woodson Junior High School is a typical junior high school, there may be resistance among "content area" teachers to the idea that they should make serious efforts at improving the reading and writing ability of their students. "It’s not my job" may be the attitude of some teachers. (See "Make It Voluntary" section below.) So, on average, junior high school students within the Virgin Islands public schools read poorly when they enter the junior high school, and they are still reading poorly when they emerge from junior high school and enter 9th grade. Can we devise an effective reading and writing improvement program that could be used both in reading classes and in English and other "content area" classes - one that can make a real difference? B. Review of Literature Make It VoluntaryMark W. Conley of Michigan State University (Conley, 1995) points out that "[b]eyond the elementary level, reading is most often taught as a separate course" (p.85). Conley states that "[i]t has ... been suggested that every teacher should be a teacher of reading (Karlan, 1984)." But Conley asserts that "content-area teachers don’t have the time or the expertise necessary to meet all of the reading needs of students in their [middle school or junior high school] classrooms" (p.90). Conley notes the "natural reluctance of content-area teachers to participate in reading programs," and claims that "Teachers who participate in any [reading] program should be volunteers. Many middle school and junior high reading programs that fail do so because programs have been thrust on content-area teachers who are not ready for them" (p.93). Let’s take Mark Conley’s advice, and propose a voluntary school improvement project. Dr. Conley claims that "content-area" teachers don’t have the time or the expertise necessary to meet all of the reading needs of students in their classrooms. What can we do to fill this gap in reading-improvement expertise? How can we carve out the time needed to make all classrooms reading classrooms, if that is what we want to do? To answer these questions, it is important to understand the elements of reading with understanding. Elements of Reading with UnderstandingJacobs (1997) has explained that there are five elements of reading with understanding: (1) the ability to "speak the written word"; (2) possession of vocabulary, including "cultural literacy"; (3) knowledge of grammar; (4) clear thought including logic; and (5) orientation. Jacobs has pointed out that elements 2 through 5 (vocabulary, grammar, clear thinking, and orientation), when it comes to reading, depend upon the first element - the ability to "speak the written word." What is "the ability to ‘speak the written word’"? It means, here, the ability to look at a page of text of written English and to say aloud, understandably to a listener, exactly what the author wrote, word-for-word. To paraphrase Jacobs, if you can’t ‘talk out loud’ the words on the page, your vocabulary, grammar, clear thinking, and orientation do not make you a reader. "A good vocabulary, an understanding of standard English grammar, the ability to think clearly, and a healthy orientation in life will all help a reader to catch the meaning of what he reads, but they all hang on the ability to carry the written word on the page into speech" (Jacobs, 1997, p.2). The informal investigation at the John H. Woodson Junior High School suggests that a principal reason many of the junior high school students cannot read at grade level is that they cannot "speak the written word" of grade-level reading materials. This could be confirmed through testing a sample of students with the Reading Competency Test, an informal reading inventory published by the National Right to Read Foundation. The author of this report suspects that an effective school improvement program will need to develop this ability to speak aloud every single word on the page with precision. A complete program would work on vocabulary, grammar, clear thinking and orientation as well. Direct InstructionWhat instructional techniques should the teachers use in the reading and writing improvement project? For one thing, in this project the participating teachers will be encouraged to learn the technique known as "direct instruction." Direct instruction is explicit, systematic, teacher-directed instruction. It involves getting the students’ attention; describing what is about to be taught; modeling for the students the learning behavior being taught; providing an opportunity for the students to perform the behavior themselves with the teacher; providing an opportunity for the students to perform the behavior on their own with feedback on performance; and providing opportunities for students to practice the behavior being taught and to generalize their use of the new behavior learned. Over the long haul (semester, academic year, grade-to-grade), a coordinated Direct Instruction program will likely systematically teach the subjects to be taught, part-by-part. Its use in this program, however, would largely be stripped of this long-term systematistic element. Direct instruction has been found to be an effective teaching method for reading instruction and for teaching other basic skills. For instance, thirteen instructional techniques for teaching reading (Smith, 1994), part of twenty-two instructional models in various basic subjects (Grossen, 1997), were compared in Project Follow Through. Project Follow Through was the largest educational experiment in United States history. It was a $1 billion project sponsored by the Federal government "designed to determine what works best in teaching disadvantaged primary school children. The ... Direct Instruction model was applied to 9,000 children at 20 sites [in the early 1970s]. In 1977, an independent firm tabulated test results and concluded that [the] program had outperformed all others. [The others included "language experience", a forerunner of today’s "whole language." (Grossen , p. 22).] For children who attended kindergarten through third grade, Direct Instruction came first in reading, arithmetic, spelling, language, basic skills, academic cognitive skills and even self-esteem. But it was never widely implemented because its approach contradicted the progressive dogma of the day" (Smith, 1994). So, direct instruction is effective. What kind of direct instruction, or other kinds of instruction, more spontaneously "tied to the moment" and less systematized, might be able to be used in an easily implemented reading and writing school improvement program that can be "plugged right into" classrooms at the John H. Woodson Junior High School and other schools? What about the first element of reading with understanding - "speaking the written word" - how should that be taught? The Importance of Phonics and Explicit PhonicsBonnie Grossen, a professor at the University of Oregon, and the editor of the journal Effective School Practices, wrote an essay entitled "The Research Base for Reading Mastery, SRA," consisting of 22 pages, plus a supplemental 12 pages entitled "References for Reading Mastery Research Review," and placed them on the World Wide Web. The Grossen quotations found here are from this website. (http://darkwing.uoregon.edu/:adiep/rdgtxt.htm) The website does not indicate when it was posted or revised. Since it is a current website, the Grossen work is referenced as Grossen (1997). Dr. Grossen points out that "focused instruction in decoding (phonics) is superior to instruction that does not provide a decoding focus" (p.2). In other words, students should be taught to "decode" the words on the page. To do this, the children need to be taught the "encoding" and "decoding" rules of written English. These are the rules of phonics - the rules explaining our alphabetic system, where letters and groups of letters symbolize the sounds of our speech. The "sound values" of individual letters (especially vowels) can vary from word to word, so the rules get a little complicated, but not too complicated. Our children are smart enough to learn the rules. If the children are exposed to the rules again and again and again and again, they will learn them. If they do learn the phonics rules well, they can stop hesitantly stumbling and hacking their way through grade-level (and beyond) reading, the way so many of them do now. Grossen (1997) makes reference to Jeanne Chall’s famous book Learning to Read: The Great Debate (Chall, 1967), a meta-analysis of reading research, which resulted in Dr. Chall’s conclusion (paraphrased by Dr. Grossen) that "focused instruction in phonics was superior to instruction without this focus in teaching students word recognition, oral reading, and spelling (Chall, 1983). These findings held for both low performers and normally-achieving students" (p.2). Grossen further reports that "Jeffrey and Samuels (1976) and Polloway, Epstein, Polloway, Patton and Ball (1986) found that a phonics program resulted in significantly better reading comprehension than other programs" (p.2). Grossen further reports that "Foorman (1995) reviewed the research on the great debate and concluded, ‘empirical evidence favors explicit instruction in alphabetic coding’ (p.388). Baker and Stahl (1994) emphasized the importance of explicitly teaching alphabetic coding" (pp. 2-3). The most famous recent meta-analysis of reading instruction, Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning About Print by Marilyn Jager Adams (Adams, 1990), concluded that direct, explicit phonics instruction is important for the development of skillful readers. Adams emphasizes that to become a good reader, the student must overlearn phonics information, explaining "It is their overlearned knowledge about the sequence of letters comprising frequent words and spelling patterns that enables skillful readers to process the letters of a text so quickly and easily. As the reader fixates each word of text, the individual letters in focus are perceived almost instantly and effortlessly. Yet even as the letters are perceived, they are automatically clustered into familiar spelling patterns by virtue of the learned association among them. Thus it is because of their deep knowledge about orthography that skillful readers look and feel as though they recognize words holistically" (p.410). Although Rudolph Flesch’s 1955 book Why Johnny Can’t Read (Flesch, 1955) was an informal polemic directed at parents rather than reading instruction professionals, it too included a review of reading instruction research, concluding that children should be taught to read by direct, explicit, systematic phonics. He followed up with another informal analysis 25 years later in Why Johnny Still Can’t Read (Flesch, 1981) confirming that conclusion. Groff (1987) also makes a powerful argument for direct, explicit phonics instruction in his Preventing Reading Failure: An Examination of the Myths of Reading Instruction. The Report of the Commission on Reading, Becoming a Nation of Readers (1985) asserted: "Thus, the issue is no longer, as it was several decades ago, whether children should be taught phonics. The issues now are specific ones of just how it should be done. (p.37) ... Which works better, then, explicit or implicit phonics? ... the trend of the data favors explicit phonics. (Citation omitted) In the judgment of the Commission, isolating the sounds associated with most letters and teaching children to blend the sound of letters together to try to identify words are useful instructional strategies. These are the strategies of explicit phonics. ... When children are encouraged to think of other words they know with similar spellings when they encounter a word they cannot readily identify, they are probably helped to develop the adult strategy of decoding unknown words by analogy with ones that are known (Citation omitted)" (Commission on Reading Report, p.42). Grossen (1997) asserts that "phonemic awareness alone is not sufficient for many children. Explicit instruction in common letter-sound correspondences is also necessary (Adams, [1990]; Ball & Blachman, 1991: Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley, 1990; Mann, 1993; Rack, Snowling, & Olson, 1992; Snowling, 1991; Spector, 1995; Stanovich, 1986; Vellutino, 1991; Vellutino & Scanlon, 1987a)" (Grossen, p.4). The United States, and the Virgin Islands with it, moved away from phonics-based reading instruction many years ago. Most "basal readers" popular over these many years, were not phonics-rule-based. To the contrary, many words were presented to the students for memorization without accompanying instructions on the phonics rules explaining their spelling. The children were not expected to think of all words (that’s right, including so-called "sight" words) as composed of letters and groups of letters symbolizing the discernable sounds of our speech. Our World of Reading, the elementary school reading instruction series used in the Virgin Islands public schools in recent years, and the new Literature Works program just put in place in Virgin Islands public elementary schools, do not seek to instill in students the habit of always using phonics rules to figure out how to "speak out loud" each written word encountered in print. These programs instead see phonics decoding (and encoding) as one of a host of "reading strategies" available to children as they go about the process of learning to read well. As a result, one would expect many students entering John H. Woodson Junior High School have developed bad reading habits. They very likely guess at words on the page because they have not been taught the habit of figuring them all out with precision. In neither the old Our World of Reading program, nor the new Literature Works program, which is founded in the philosophy of "whole language," are the teachers encouraged to inundate the children with the phonics information they need to decode every word with precision. Instead, the children are permitted, and even often encouraged, to use any-old "reading strategy." As articulated by Emerald Dechant in his Whole-Language Reading (A Comprehensive Teaching Guide) [Dechant, 1993, p. 109]: The evidence .. indicates that it is difficult to become a skilled reader without mastering the conventionalized code that maps the writing system to speech. Young children with difficulty in reading almost invariably have imperfect knowledge of the coding system. Perfetti (1985) observes that "readers of low ability have inefficient - slow and effortful - coding as the major obstacle to reading achievement" (p. 10). Nevertheless, successful reading depends upon increasingly less expenditure of effort and energy on the coding skills and a greater expenditure of energy on the increased demands of the text and on the application of higher-level comprehension processes. Coding is not less useful as the pupil matures in reading; its use is more refined. A major concern today is the treatment of errors in word identification and in recoding. It is a common practice to downplay errors or miscues that maintain the "approximate meaning." If proficiency and accuracy in oral reading are a legitimate goal in reading, then it seems an equally valid goal to have the pupil develop accuracy and indeed automaticity in pronouncing the words on the printed page. Fluency in reading, and certainly reading orally to others (and presumably sharing a message with others orally) requires this. Unfortunately, as the Virgin Islands moves out of one elementary school basal reader system which was not phonics-based and into another founded on the whole language philosophy, students entering John H. Woodson Junior High School in the years to come may be just as bad as many of them now probably are at decoding well, since the whole language philosophy downplays the importance of phonics decoding in reading. Emerald Dechant (1993, p. 164), again: Unfortunately, in reading the whole-language literature one is likely to find only cursory lip service given to the within-word cues [use of phonics rules] in general and to the graphophonemic cues [more phonics rules] in particular. There is almost none, or only fleeting attention given to helping children develop automaticity in recoding. This state of affairs is not necessarily the elementary teacher’s fault. The real tragedy is that too many elementary school teachers never learned how the alphabetic orthography maps onto the language, why beginning readers must understand how the internal structure of word relates to the orthography, and why it is difficult for children to understand this (Liberman and Shankweiler, 1991, p. 13). Too many teachers have not been taught how to improve the pupil’s predictive strategy in reading by use of the within-word cues [phonics rules]; have not learned how to develop automaticity in recoding [mastery of phonics rules]; and, unfortunately, emerge from their professional education program with only a superficial knowledge of how to break the alphabetic code, and with even less understanding of how to help children to beak the code. Too many professors in colleges of education simply fail to teach prospective elementary school teachers how to recode. Too frequently, prospective teachers and practicing teachers in workshops, institutes, conferences, and professional articles are encouraged not to trouble children with how the orthography works. They are taught only how to teach children to use "the known words as a basis for guessing the rest of the message from picture cues and context" (Liberman and Shankweiler, 1991, p. 14). It is indeed fortunate that a sizable number of children learn the alphabetic principle on their own. These children generally are strong in phonological processing. However, the children with phonological deficits do not understand that spoken words consist of phonemes, and have not learned that there is a correspondence between the phonemes and the graphemes of the printed word. For these children, ... the psycholinguistic guessing-game and phonics-free whole-language approaches "are likely to be disastrous. Children taught this way are likely to join the ranks of the millions of functional illiterates in our country who stumble along, guessing at the printed message from their little store of memorized words, unable to decipher a new word they have never seen before" (Liberman and Shankweiler, 1991, p. 14). It is a truism that "it is much easier to describe how to teach reading than to state specifically how children learn this process" (Heilman, 1961), but one thing is certain - the teacher needs to fill more slots in the pupil’s schemata with information about within-word cues [phonics rules]. [Bracketed material added by Jacobs.] Dechant (1993, pp. 179-180) further explains: Perfetti (1985) adds: "A child who learns to code has knowledge that can enable him to read no matter how the semantic, syntactic, and pragmatic cues might conspire against him. No matter how helpful they are to reading, these [latter] cues are not really a substitute for the ability to identify a word" (p. 239). He continues: "It has yet to be demonstrated that there are individuals who have comprehension strategy deficits without [recoding] fluency problems" (p. 244). Perfetti (1985) notes that "the orthographic system provides a constrained set of possibilities for any given string of letters. And the coding principles provide very narrow choices for any orthographic string. Of course, lead may map onto led or lead out of context. But it can’t map onto window or deer. The skilled reader has adequate knowledge to identify almost all words with very minimal context. For the skilled reader, reading is psycholinguistic, but it is no guessing game" (p. 9). In the view of these researchers the graphophonic cue system is an important and coequal cueing system with the semantic and syntactic context, and in skilled readers it often becomes the primary one. They hold that semantics, syntactics, and pragmatics alone are not adequate to the task of recoding and reading. From our perspective, the almost total reliance by college faculties, elementary teachers, and reading methods textbooks on context cues, semantics, syntactics, and pragmatics, and the failure to help children in the use of the within-word cues [Jacobs: phonics rules] in making focal predictions in reading have been one of the major deficiencies in reading education in the last twenty-five years. The "conscious-prediction-from-context" strategy has simply not worked. The psycholinguistic guessing-game hypothesis has unfortunately led both teachers and pupils down the path of failure. It has led to aberrations in defining psycholinguistics, an overemphasis of the meaning cues, a disastrous disparaging of the within-word cues (the graphic-logographic-orthographic-phonological-graphophonic cues), failure to help readers to understand the alphabetic principle, rejection of code instruction in reading, and indeed to a misinterpretation of the psycholinguistic nature of reading. It has led to the promulgation of "psycholinguistic methods" and "psycholinguistic materials," and is a least to some degree responsible for the failure of thousands of children and millions of adults to achieve literacy. It is a major reason why children today are not reading better than they read twenty-five years ago, why many do not learn to read, and why countless others are not reading as well as they might. Unfortunately, many of those who are today leaders of the whole-language movement are the very same people who have promulgated the psycholinguistic guessing-game hypothesis. Grossen (1997) further points out that "Pany and McCoy (1988) found that third-grade children with reading disabilities who made a large number of errors during reading (10%-15%) significantly improved their word recognition and comprehension scores when given immediate feedback on every single error. When corrective feedback was provided after every error, the children made significantly fewer errors overall, significantly fewer meaning-change errors during reading of the passage, significantly fewer errors on lists of error words presented on an immediate and delayed bases, and significantly fewer errors on passage-comprehension questions. Simply receiving feedback on errors that altered the meaning of the passages had no effect" (p.10). Teaching Blending, Letter Sounds, and Sounding OutGrossen (1997) further reports that "Pupils should be taught how to blend sounds together into words. Coleman (1970) noted that blending is a strategy that students can apply to many different words, but direct instruction in the blending strategy using many sounds is necessary before students will acquire the generalized skill. Skailand (1971) and Silberman (1964) reported that if subjects are taught sound-symbol relationships but not blending, they will not use sounding out as a decoding strategy. Bishop (1964), Jeffrey and Samuels (1967), Carnine (1977), and Vandever and Neville (1976) reported that teaching letter-sound correspondences and sounding out resulted in students’ correctly identifying more unfamiliar words than when students were trained on a whole-word strategy. Haddock (1976) and Chapman and Kamm (1974) found that only when blending is directly taught will students successfully use a sounding-out strategy for attacking words" (p.7). Direct Instruction in Word Meanings (Vocabulary)Grossen (1997) further reports that "Teaching vocabulary concepts explicitly has a positive effect on comprehension and on the ability to use context to learn new words. Of course, direct instruction of vocabulary can teach no more than a fraction of the words that students need to learn during their K-12 years. However, direct teaching can have significant effects not only on comprehension of passages containing taught words, but also on comprehension in general and on the ability to learn new words in context. Beck, Perfetti, and McKeown (1982) found that students who were given direct instruction in word meanings were better able to discern meanings of untaught words than control subjects. Stahl and Fairbanks (1986) suggest that teaching 350 words each year may augment learning from context by 10 to 30%, a significant amount" (p.14). Explicit Instruction in LogicGrossen (1997) further reports that "Reasoning deductively (logically) is difficult for all populations and can be improved with explicit instruction. Research has documented in all populations that the most common error in reasoning is to form a conclusion without sufficient evidence (Ceraso & Provitera, 1971; Grossen, 1991; Grossen & Carnine, 1990; Grossen, Lee, & Johnston, 1995; Simpson & Johnson, 1966). Direct instruction in syllogistic reasoning can have a positive impact on these error patterns (Grossen & Carnine, 1990; Grossen, Lee & Johnson, 1995). These effects transfer to other critical thinking and reasoning activities (Grossen, 1991; Grossen, Lee & Johnston, 1995)" (p.19). Logan (1986) contends that as children learn implicitly our alphabetic system, they learn abstraction, analysis, rationality, and classification "which form the essence of the alphabet effect and the basis for Western abstract scientific and logical thinking" (p. 21). Orton PhonogramsMany people have devised programs for teaching to read and write through phonics. One of those was Samuel T. Orton, M.D. a neuropathologist. He wrote the 1937 book Reading, Writing and Speech Problems in Children (Baltimore: The Orton Society for Dyslexia). Out of his original work the Orton-Gillingham method for teaching slow learners to read well through phonics was developed. He developed the 70 Orton phonograms for correct spelling, which groups written English spellings based upon the sounds they represent. Usefulness of Dictation; the Spalding Spelling RulesRomalda Spalding was a teacher who worked under Dr. Orton. She developed her own phonics-based reading instruction method, explained in her 1957 (revised 1991) book The Writing Road to Reading, which includes her 28 Spalding spelling rules and additional phonograms. As the title of her book implies, Mrs. Spalding advocated the teaching of reading through writing. Basically, the approach involves the teaching of phonics rules based upon the 70 Orton phonograms and the 28 Spalding spelling rules. Initially, children are given dictation and they write out the letters, syllables, and words based upon the phonics rules they have been taught. This method of teaching reading is promoted by The Riggs Institute of Beaverton, Oregon, which The Riggs Institute reports is highly effective. For instance, on The Riggs Institute website, the Institute presents a bar graph of Arizona pupil achievement testing using the 1985 Iowa test of basic skills, comparing total language scores of schools using the Spalding method, compared to statewide and national scores, demonstrating marked superiority of the the Spalding method over the general statewide and national population. The extensive Riggs Institute website may be accessed through a "phonics" search using Yahoo or Infoseek or other search engines on the Internet. Statement of the Hypothesis The author of this school improvement plan hypothesizes that a well-structured plan can be devised for use by reading classroom teachers and "content area" teachers, utilizing phonics-based reading instruction, including dictation and correction of oral reading, together with direct instruction in vocabulary, grammar, and logical thinking, which would result in a measurable bettering of the reading abilities of the students who are the subjects of the program. II. The Phonics Institute"Every Teacher a Reading Teacher" Reading and Writing School Improvement Program Description of the ProposalThe author of this school improvement project proposal suggests that the John H. Woodson Junior High School contract with The Phonics Institute to find the funding for, to develop, to put in place, and to supervise at the John H. Woodson Junior High School (and other Virgin Islands public schools desiring to participate in the program) the "Every Teacher a Reading Teacher" reading and writing school improvement project. The "Every Teacher a Reading Teacher" program would be developed by The Phonics Institute and would include the following elements: The Phonics Institute would develop and design classroom displays (and other classroom material) that would display phonics rules including a sound chart, the 70 Orton phonograms, the Spalding phonograms, and the 28 Spalding rules of spelling. The Phonics Institute would also develop and design other displays of various rules of grammar and inflection; principles of logic and clear thinking; the elements of reading with understanding; the system of direct instruction; word roots, prefixes, and suffixes; and vocabulary word lists (which, depending upon the amount of display space available, could include up to perhaps 1,000 or more of the most common words). The smallest classrooms at John H. Woodson Junior High School are approximately 20' x 20' and there is a minimum of 50 to 55 linear horizontal feet for the mounting of displays in the space approximate 2' high from the top of the blackboard to the ceiling. C After designing the program, The Phonics Institute would make a "pitch" to the John H. Woodson High School teachers, counselors, and administrators, explaining the program and encouraging voluntary participation. C The Phonics Institute would train all teachers participating in the program, whether "reading" teachers or "content area" teachers. The training could consist of one-on-one meetings, group meetings of participating teachers, written materials, and video tapes. Training would include instruction in "Direct Instruction" sequences and techniques and how they can be used with the dictation and oral reading involved in the program. C The elements of the classroom program have not yet been "set in concrete" and the elements listed here can be expanded, contracted, or otherwise altered, but one basic format of the program would include the following: ~ The classrooms of all participating teachers would be outfitted with the displays, and the teachers would be provided with approximately three-foot-long wooden pointers. ~ Each participating teacher would prepare prior to each classroom session a written selection, consisting of a sentence, a few sentences, or a paragraph, which normally would be about the "content area" being taught in the class. The teacher at the beginning of class (after attendance or whatever other preliminary matters are handled) would dictate the written selection to the class and all students would be required to write-out the selection and immediately hand it in to the teacher. The teacher could then quickly review the students’ written work and make reference to the displays (using a pointer) and use the blackboard to explore with the children the phonics rules (or grammatical rules as the case may be) that explain how the words should have been written, if the students have made any errors. The teacher should attempt to write out the selections at the "instructional level," so that some errors, but not too many errors would be expected to show up in the students’ writings. ~ The participating teachers would be encouraged to have students read aloud from the "content area" reading materials during the class. When a student makes a mistake in oral reading, again the teacher would use the pointer, the displays, and the blackboard, to explicitly explore with the students the rules governing the pronunciation of the words and grammatical structures which have given problems to the student reading aloud. The roots, prefixes, and suffixes list and the vocabulary list can also be used as a reference for explicit discussion of the meaning of the words in the text. ~ In exploring with the students’ analysis, synthesis, interpretation, and the like of the text being read, again the teacher can make reference to the rules of logic and clear thinking on display in the classroom in order heighten the awareness of the students to their own thinking processes. C Ideally, the author of this report, as the director of The Phonics Institute, would be involved initially with the teacher in explaining to the students in the class the nature and purposes of this reading improvement program so that the students understand exactly what is being done and why. C Ideally, the author of this proposal, as the director of The Phonics Institute, would periodically visit classrooms participating in the program in order to monitor the program in the actual classroom settings in order to assist the teacher in making the most effective use of the program. C Ideally, each student in participating classrooms would be given his or her own miniature copy of the classroom displays for personal use while reading and writing. C Ideally, the class would keep track of those phonics, grammar, and logical rules that individual members of the class and that the class as a whole has mastered in order to foster a sense of progress towards the final goal of mastery of all rules. C Ideally, the program would be accompanied by a system of checking all, or representative samples, of the written material (including tests, other handouts, and the like) produced by the participating teachers to their classes, and produced by students in tests and written assignments, to ensure that those materials are written in "proper" English. C Ideally, all classes participating in the program (or perhaps only those classes where the teachers have been "certified" by The Phonics Institute as competent users of the program) would be pre- and post-tested for their reading and writing abilities, to assist in assessing the effectiveness of the program. The basic concept behind the proposal is to make use of phonics, grammar, and other rules, constantly on display in the classroom, to correct errors displayed in student written response to teacher dictation in class and in student oral reading in class. This will allow focusing in on the "decoding" (reading) and "encoding" (spelling) problems that the students have. The display of rules of grammar and logic, in conjunction with dictation and oral reading, will provide many quick opportunities for learning how to better read and write. The program is well grounded in research-based pedagogical principles of teaching reading, writing, vocabulary, grammar, and thinking skills. There may be some resistance on the part of the teachers - both "reading" teachers and "content area" teachers for a variety of reasons. The author of this proposal, as the director of The Phonics Institute, would do his best to persuade teachers to participate in the program. If there is too much resistance at John H. Woodson Junior High School (and even if there isn’t), the program can be easily instituted in other classrooms at the elementary, junior high school, high school, and university level. The program should be a good tool for the improvement of reading and writing. The use of explicit, extensive phonics rules, frequently referenced, together with direct instruction in blending and syllabication have proven to be effective means for improving reading. The use of dictation with immediate corrections making reference to explicit displayed rules should help spelling as well. The use of frequent oral reading with immediate corrections and reference to the phonics rules again should help the students to improve their reading abilities. The use of perpetual displays in the classroom will make the process fairly quick and easy, which will cut down on the problem of carving out time from normal classroom activities. With frequent use of the display, dictation, and oral reading, the need for use of the displays and the procedures should lessen. Ultimately, because the children will be reading better, it is not anticipated that the program will make it significantly more difficult to cover all "content area" subjects. Rather, with the children reading better things should go more quickly and more easily ultimately. The program has the advantages that the teachers will learn phonics, the rules of grammar, and the rules of logic, and the students will as well. Another advantage of the program is that it is teacher-intensive, with all classroom activities being teacher-directed. Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Baker, S. & Stahl, S. (1994). Beginning reading: Educational tools for diverse learners. School Psychology Review, 23(3), 372-394. Ball, E.W., & Blachman, B.A. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and development spelling? Reading Research Quarterly, 26(1), 49-66. Beck, I., Perfetti, C., & McKeown, M.G. (l982). The effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 482-502. Bishop, C. (l964). Transfer effects of word and letter training in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 204-221. Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1989). Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 313-321. Carnine, D. (1977). Phonics versus look-say: Transfer to new words. Reading Teacher, 30(6), 636-640. Ceraso, J., & Provitera, A. (1971). Sources of error in syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 400-410. Chall, J.S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill. Chall, J.S. (1983a). Learning to read: The great debate. Updated ed. New York: McGraw-Hill. Chapman, R., & Kamm, M. (1974). An evaluation of methods for teaching initial sound isolation. (ERIC Document ED 066-231). Coleman, E. (1970). Collecting a data base for a reading technology. Journal of Educational Psychology Monograph, 61(4, Part 2). Commission on Reading. (1985). Becoming a nation of readers: The report of the commission on reading. Washington, D.C.: The National Institute of Education. Conley, M.W. (1995). Middle school and junior high school reading programs. In Wepner, S.B., Feeley, J.P. & Strickland, D.S. (Eds.) The administration and supervision of reading programs (2nd ed.). New York: International Reading Association. Dechant, E. (1993). Whole-language reading: A comprehensive teaching guide. Lancaster: Technomic Publishing AG. Flesch, R. (1955). Why johnny can't read - And what you can do about it. New York: HarperCollins. Flesch, R. (1981). Why johnny still can't read - A new look at the scandal of our schools. New York: HarperCollins. Foorman, B. (1995). Research on "the great debate": Code-oriented versus whole language approaches to reading instruction. School Psychology Review, 24(3), 376-392. Groff, P. (1987). Preventing reading failure: An examination of the myths of reading instruction. Portland: National Book Company. Grossen, B. (1997). The research base for Reading mastery, SRA. World Wide Web: (http://darkwing.uoregon.edu/:adiep/rdgtxt.htm). Grossen, B. (1991). The fundamental skills of higher order thinking. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24(6), 343-353. Grossen, B., & Carnine, D. (1990). Diagraming a logic strategy: Effects on difficult problem types and transfer. Learning Disability Quarterly, 13(2), 168-182. Grossen, B., & Lee, C., & Johnston, D. (1995). Comparison of the effects of direct instruction in reasoning with constructivism on deductive reasoning. Oregon Monograph, 78, 253-274. Haddock, M. (1976). Effects of an auditory and an auditory-visual method of blending instruction on the ability of prereaders to decode synthetic words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 825-831. Jacobs, E.H. (1997). Proposal to design and put in place voluntary phonics-based reading programs in virgin islands public schools (March 31, 1997). This proposal was made March 31, 1997 to the Virgin Islands Education Commission and the Virgin Islands Literacy Task Force, and is on file with the library at the University of the Virgin Islands, St. Croix Campus. Jeffrey, W., & Samuels, S. (1976). Effect of method of reading training on initial learning and transfer. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 354-358. Logan, R.K. (1986). The alphabet effect: The impact of the phonetic alphabet on the development of western civilization. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Mann, V.A. (1993). Phoneme awareness and future reading ability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(4), 259-269. National Commission on Excellence in Education. (1983). A nation at risk: The imperative for educational reform (Stock No. 065-000-00177-2). Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. Orton, S.T. (M.D.) (1937). Reading, writing and speech problems in children. Baltimore: The Orton Society for Dyslexia. Pany, D., & McCoy, K. (1988). Effects of corrective feedback on word accuracy and reading comprehension of readers with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21(9), 546-550. Polloway, E., Epstein, M., Poloway, C., Patton, J., & Ball, D. (1986). Corrective reading program: An analysis of effectiveness with learning disabled and mentally retarded students. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 41-47. Rack, J.P., Snowling, M.J., & Olson, R.K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(1), 29-53. Silberman, H. (1964). Exploratory research on a beginning reading program. Santa Monica, CA: System Development Corporation. Skailand, D. (1971). A comparison of four language units in teaching beginning reading. Paper presented to the American Educational Research Association, New York. Smith, C. (1994). Blackboard jungle. Effective School Practices, 13(4), 33-36. Spalding, R., & Spalding W. (1957, 1981). The writing road to reading. New York: William Morrow. Spector, J.E. (1995). Phonemic awareness training: Application of principles of direct instruction. Reading & Writing Quarterly, II, 37-51. Stahl, S., & Fairbanks, M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 5, 72-110. Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 360-407. Vandever, T., & Neville, D. (1976). Transfer as a result of synthetic and analytic reading instruction. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 30, 498-503. Vellutino, F.R. (1991). Introduction to three studies on reading acquisition: Convergent findings on theoretical foundations of code-oriented versus whole-language approaches to reading instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(4), 437-443. Vellutino, F.R., & Scanlon, D.M. (1987a). Linguistic coding and reading ability. In S. Rosenberg (Ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics (pp. 1-69). New York: Cambridge University Press. 4/16/97\\Sandee\WPDOCS\Ned's Assistant\Phonics Institute,The\Every Teacher a Reading Teacher.wpd
|
www.phonicsinstitute.com |